Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Show
Recommended textbook solutions

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Social Psychology

10th EditionElliot Aronson, Robin M. Akert, Samuel R. Sommers, Timothy D. Wilson

525 solutions

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Social Psychology

10th EditionElliot Aronson, Robin M. Akert, Timothy D. Wilson

525 solutions

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Social Psychology

10th EditionElliot Aronson, Robin M. Akert, Samuel R. Sommers, Timothy D. Wilson

525 solutions

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Social Psychology and Human Nature

5th EditionBrad J. Bushman, Roy F. Baumeister

374 solutions

  • Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression
    Access through your institution

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Research suggests that men are more likely than women to engage in indirect aggression

Abstract

The wealth of literature indicating that men are more physically aggressive than women may be the reason for the dearth of research on physical aggression in women. However, recent research has found that this discrepancy is better attributed to conformity to gender roles rather than to biological sex. The purpose of the present study was to assess the influence of masculine and feminine gender role conformity on direct physical aggression in women. One-hundred eighty four women were recruited to compete in a sham aggression paradigm against either a hyperfeminine (gender role conforming) or hypofeminine (gender role violating) confederate woman. Results indicated that women evinced more physical aggression toward a hypofeminine woman than toward her hyperfeminine counterpart. Moreover, endorsement of masculine traits by female aggressors was positively correlated with behavioral and self-reports of physical aggression. Femininity was unrelated to behavioral indices of physical aggression but negatively correlated with self-reports of aggression. Findings are discussed in reference to the increased risk of victimization of women who deviate from gender-specific role norms and the impact of perpetrator gender role adherence on aggressive behavior. Future examination of differential effects of general nonconformity on aggression as an alternative explanation of the findings is required.

Introduction

“Although gender is a very attractive and frequently examined variable among those who are interested in understanding aggression, its appeal may be greater than its explanatory power” (Richardson & Hammock, 2007; p. 418). Gender effects, particularly as they pertain to the perpetration of aggression, have been examined extensively with multiple methodologies and experimental designs, across disparate populations and settings, and on varying types of aggressive behavior. Results from crime statistics (FBI Uniform Crime Reports, 2007), self-report data (Burton, Hafetz, & Henninger, 2007), behavioral experiments (Gussler-Burkhardt and Giancola, 2005, Verona et al., 2007), developmental research (Crick and Grotpeter, 1995, Salmivalli and Kaukiainen, 2004), and comprehensive meta-analyses (Bettencourt and Miller, 1996, Frodi et al., 1977) have consistently indicated that men evince more direct physical aggression relative to women (Richardson & Hammock, 2007). For example crime statistics indicate that men commit more than half of all homicides, are the predominant perpetrators of rape and domestic violence against women, and are the typical perpetrators of antigay aggression (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2007, National Coalition of Anti-Violence Programs, 2007). In the laboratory, men administer more shocks to a confederate at greater intensity, and administer the highest available shock at greater proportions than women. In contrast, women demonstrate a longer latency before becoming aggressive and initiate aggression at lower intensities than men (Zeichner, Parrott, & Frey, 2003). This pattern of gender differences in the aggression literature is likely the reason that physical aggression is sparsely studied in women relative to men.

Researchers have also attempted to address the unique effects of victim gender on the occurrence of aggressive acts. Harris examined gender differences in victimization of aggression and found that men were more likely to have been the target of more forms of aggression (Harris, 1992), that men reported perpetrating more aggression toward men than women (Harris, 1995), and that men had experienced more aggression than women over their lifetime (Harris, 1996). Richardson and Green (1999) examined gender effects on direct aggression in same-gender and opposite-gender dyads. The authors found that both men and women endorsed more frequent incidents of direct aggression toward men than toward women, indicating that gender of target influences aggressive behavior. Basow, Cahill, Phelan, Longshore, and McGillicuddy-DeLisi (2007) explored gender effects on perceptions of aggressive behavior and found that physical aggression toward women was rated as less acceptable and more harmful than when the target was a man. As a whole, extant data would seem to suggest that men are more likely than women to be perpetrators and victims of aggressive behavior.

However, in a recent review of the literature on gender and aggression, Richardson and Hammock (2007) note that the apparent gender differences in aggressive behavior may actually reflect a response to one’s gender role. Gender role orientation represents the extent to which a person conforms to masculine or feminine norms associated with the respective gender by demonstrating socially prescribed attitudes, beliefs, and behavior congruent with being “masculine” or “feminine.” Several studies have investigated the unique influence of gender role orientation on aggression. For example, Hammock and Richardson (1992) examined effects of both gender and gender role orientation on aggressive behavior in a laboratory aggression paradigm. Although both sex and gender role, particularly masculinity, predicted aggressiveness, masculinity predicted aggressiveness better than gender, suggesting that gender role has a greater impact on aggressive behavior than gender alone. Kogut, Langley, and O’Neal (1992) found that women who endorsed a high level of masculinity were more aggressive than their nonmasculine counterparts. Gini and Pozzoli (2006) explored the association between gender role and children’s bullying. The authors found that femininity was unrelated to bullying in both boys and girls, while a strong association between bullying and masculinity was found in boys, (r = .62, p < .001) and in girls, (r = .30, p < .05). Moreover, when gender was entered with gender role (i.e., masculinity and femininity) into a hierarchical regression analysis, masculinity explained approximately 16% of the variance, while gender and femininity did not explain an appreciable proportion of variance. Walker, Richardson, and Green (2000) investigated the effects of gender role on direct and indirect aggressive tendencies in a sample of elderly men and women (Mage = 71). They found that masculinity was positively associated with reported indirect aggression while femininity was negatively associated with reported direct aggression. Likewise, Kinney, Smith, and Donzella (2001) explored effects of gender role on expression of anger and verbal aggressiveness in a group of collegiate men and women. Results indicated that masculinity was positively correlated with expression of anger as well as verbal aggressiveness, whereas femininity was negatively correlated with both. Taken as a whole, results of these studies highlight the influence of gender role conformity on an individual’s endorsement and expression of aggression, and they suggest that masculine gender role conformity is associated with increased perpetration of aggression. However, to date, the study of effects of feminine gender role conformity on aggression has been equivocal and warrants further exploration.

In reference to the effect of the victims’ gender role conformity on aggression, studies have primarily focused on men’s role violations. Moreover, these studies have typically utilized “extreme” violations of gender roles such as depiction of homosexual behavior. The totality of the data indicate that men who endorse traditional masculine beliefs are more aggressive toward gay men (e.g., Bernat et al., 2001, Parrott and Peterson, 2008, Parrott et al., 2008). Rarely has the effect of women’s role violations been studied in the laboratory. In one study, Parrott and Gallagher (2008) found that women who conform to traditional gender roles experience increased anger after viewing video depicting female gender role violations. Again, this manipulation involved “extreme” violations demonstrated by viewing sexual acts by same sex dyads. To our knowledge, no published studies have examined effects of gender role violations without using sexual violations in women.

The purpose of the present study was to replicate and expand upon previous findings by examining effects of perpetrator and victim gender role conformity on women’s direct physical aggression. To do this we employed a laboratory shock paradigm. In reference to femininity, the literature provides mixed support for its relation to aggression. The majority of literature reviewed yielded no relation between femininity and aggression. Although two studies found a significant negative correlation (i.e., Kinney et al., 2001, Walker et al., 2000), neither of these studies used behavioral measures of physical aggression. Moreover, Hammock and Richardson (1992) who utilized a similar aggression paradigm found no relation between femininity and behavioral indices of physical aggression, despite finding a negative association with self-reported aggression. For these reasons, we did not expect a significant relationship between femininity and physical aggression in the present study. However, we hypothesized that femininity would negatively correlate with self-reports of aggression. We expected a significant positive relationship between perpetrator masculinity and all indices of aggression. With regard to gender role conformity of the target, we hypothesized that a hypofeminine woman (i.e., role violating) would elicit more aggression relative to a hyperfeminine (role conforming) woman. This hypothesis is based on research suggesting that women become angry in response to role violating women (e.g., Parrott & Gallagher, 2008). Finally, we expected that positive correlation between perpetrator masculinity and physical aggression would be significantly larger when used against a hypofeminine target relative to a hyperfeminine target.

Section snippets

Participants and experimental design

One-hundred eighty four undergraduate women (Mage = 18.8; Meducation = 13.9 years; 76.5% Caucasians; modal income = $70,000) participated in this study. Four women chose not to participate in the study after learning they could be shocked electrically. The remaining women were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. Women in the “hyperfeminine” condition (n = 90) believed they were competing against a woman who conforms to feminine norms while women in the “hypofeminine” condition (n = 90) believed

Results

RCAP manipulation check. Participants were asked to report their impression of their opponent and her “fairness” during the task; how they felt about administering shocks, and whether they recognized their opponent’s voice. Additionally, participant behaviors such as cursing at the opponent or exclaiming the opponent’s name were observed by the experimenter via video camera and considered affirmation of the deception’s success. Of 180 participants, 21 indicated that they did not believe the

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine the influence of perpetrator and victim gender role conformity on direct physical aggression in women. In reference to the effect of perpetrator’s gender role conformity, masculinity was significantly and positively associated with all indices of aggression, which is consistent with previous research (e.g., Hammock & Richardson, 1992). Additionally, femininity was unrelated to perpetration of physical aggression but was negatively associated with

References (33)

  • et al.

    Social context of human aggression: Are we paying too much attention to gender?

    Aggression and violent behavior

    (2007)

  • S.A. Basow et al.

    Perceptions of relational aggression and physical aggression among college students: Effects of gender of perpetrator, target, and perceiver

    Psychology of Women Quarterly

    (2007)

  • J.A. Bernat et al.

    Homophobia and physical aggression toward homosexual and heterosexual individuals

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2001)

  • B.A. Bettencourt et al.

    Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1996)

  • L.A. Burton et al.

    Gender differences in relational and physical aggression

    Social Behavior and Personality

    (2007)

  • A.H. Buss et al.

    The aggression questionnaire

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1992)

  • M. Carlson et al.

    Evidence for a general construct of aggression

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1989)

  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment

    Child Development

    (1995)

  • R.E. Dobash et al.

    Violence against wives

    (1979)

  • Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2007). Uniform crime reports for the United States, 2006. U.S. Department of Justice,...

  • A. Frodi et al.

    Are women always less aggressive that men? A review of the experimental literature

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1977)

  • G. Gini et al.

    The role of masculinity in children’s bullying

    Sex Roles

    (2006)

  • N.L. Gussler-Burkhardt et al.

    Journal of Studies on Alcohol

    (2005)

  • G.S. Hammock et al.

    Predictors of aggressive behavior

    Aggressive Behavior

    (1992)

  • M.B. Harris

    Sex, race, and experiences of aggression

    Aggressive Behavior

    (1992)

  • M.B. Harris

    Ethnicity, gender and evaluations of aggression

    Aggressive Behavior

    (1995)

  • Cited by (32)

    • Evaluating a gender transformative violence prevention program for middle school boys: A pilot study

      2019, Children and Youth Services Review

      Individuals who do not share these values demonstrate gender inequitable attitudes, which are also closely related to adherence to traditional masculine norms (Murnen, 2015; Walker, Tokar, & Fischer, 2000). Traditional masculine norms are socially and culturally prescribed expectations that boys and men should be tough, strong, dominant, fearless, sexually promiscuous, risk takers, emotional stoic, homophobic, and aggressive (Reidy, Berke, Gentile, & Zeichner, 2014; Reidy, Sloan, & Zeichner, 2009). Boys and men who are higher in gender inequitable attitudes and adherence to traditional masculine norms are more likely to perpetrate SDV than individuals who are not high in these attitudes (McCauley et al., 2014; Reed, Silverman, Raj, Decker, & Miller, 2011; Reidy et al., 2015; Vagi et al., 2013).

    • Emotion dysregulation as an underlying mechanism of impulsive aggression: Reviewing empirical data to inform treatments for veterans who perpetrate violence

      2017, Aggression and Violent Behavior

      Men and women engage in about equal rates of partner violence (Archer, 2000; Stappenbeck et al., 2014; Teten, Sherman, & Han, 2009); however, men perpetrate more violence against other men and more sexual violence (Black et al., 2011). Higher levels of masculine traits are related to increased aggression in both men and women (Reidy, Sloan, & Zeichner, 2009), thus the degree to which one ascribes to traditional gender norms may influence what topics to cover in treatment. Providers working with male veterans or masculine clients may focus on reducing physical aggression and exploration of what “being a man” or “tough” means without violence, including the gender norm of chivalry.

    Arrow Up and RightView all citing articles on Scopus

    View full text

    Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Is indirect aggression typical of females?

    The social structure of the peer groups also was studied. The principal finding was that girls made greater use of indirect means of aggression, whereas the boys tended to employ direct means. Gender differences in verbal aggression were less pronounced.

    Which gender is more aggressive males or females?

    Moreover, in several studies of aggression paradigms, men showed higher aggression than women under relatively neutral conditions; however, provocation rather weakens the difference in aggressive behavior between men and women [3,13,20].

    Are males and females equally likely to engage in physical aggression?

    In laboratory studies, women are less aggressive than men, but provocation attenuates this difference. In the real world, women are just as likely to aggress against their romantic partner as men are, but men cause more serious physical and psychological harm.

    Why do males tend to be more aggressive than females?

    Sexual-selection theory says that males are competing for reproductive success, so are more aggressive generally and especially to other males. It's human nature. Social-role theory says that differences are sociological, based on traditional divisions of labour.